|
Impressions From an EventWe have feelings about actor, behavior, object, and setting when a social event begins. The pre-event feelings could be drawn from our sentiments about the event's elements, or they could be feelings that developed as a result of recent happenings. The occurring event changes pre-event feelings to new feelings, as shown in this diagram. (Setting is dropped in this diagram for simplicity.) For example, suppose we observe an employer cheats an employee. Initially we might feel positive toward the employer and employee, and feel that an act of cheating is very bad. Seeing the employer cheat the employee makes the employer seem very bad. The event causes neutralization of the employee, too, as if we have to reserve judgment about this employee under the presumption that the individual might deserve victimization. The act of cheating remains bad in the context of this event, but not as bad as usual, as if its evilness gets bounded by the mundaneness of the workplace circumstances. Impression-Formation ProcessesBoth pre-event feelings and the impressions emerging from the event can be represented as EPA measurements. Social psychologists have developed equations for accurately predicting the EPA numbers representing outcome impressions from the set of EPA numbers representing pre-event feelings.
Impression-formation equations consist of many terms. Each term represents a mental process that occurs while interpreting events. StabilityEvery impression-formation equation has a stability term. That fact means that the mind always transfers some pre-event feeling toward an event element on an EPA dimension to the post-event feeling involving the same event element on the same EPA dimension. For example, we have a tendency to see actors as good after events if the actors were good to begin with, and we tend to see actors as bad after events if they were bad before the event.
MoralityEvaluation of an actor's behavior strongly influences the impression of the actor's goodness or badness after an event. For example, anyone rescuing another gets evaluative credit for engaging in a noble act. Anyone killing another is discredited for engaging in a horrible act. The morality effect also is evident in the case of behaviors, except then it amounts to a stability effect (i.e., good behaviors remain good, bad behaviors remain bad). The morality effect gets involved in some other impression-formation processes, too, but only in small ways that will not be mentioned here.
Consistency EffectsConsistency effects relate feelings on an EPA dimension regarding two different event elements. For example, an actor who performs a bad action on a good object person violates a consistency principle - that good objects require good treatment, and that bad objects require bad treatment - so the actor seems bad not only because of the morality effect but additionally because of behavior-object inconsistency.
CongruenciesCongruency effects relate feelings on two different EPA dimensions regarding two different event elements.
Balance EffectsBalance effects relate feelings on an EPA dimension regarding all three core event elements - actor, behavior, and object. The set of feelings is balanced if all three are positive, or if any two are negative. For example, actors seem extra good if an event combines a positive actor, a positive behavior, and a positive object person.
Other EffectsThe effects considered above have major impacts on impressions formed during social events, and most of these effects have been found operative in different societies (U.S.A., Canada, Japan). Other less important effects have been found in research studies, too. These other effects will not be discussed here. Copular AssertionsImpression formation research in ACT involves action events, as just discussed, and copular assertions (e.g., the father is angry) studied as modifier-identity combinations (the angry father). In the U.S.A. modifiers have more impact on the result than does the identity, roughly two to one. A consistency effect operates such that good modifiers with good identities seem especially good. Cross-Cultural VariationsImpression-formation equations have been developed for actor-behavior-object events in U.S., Canadian, and Japanese studies, and equations for actor-behavior-object-setting events have been developed in the U.S.A. and Japan. Copular impressions have been studied in the U.S.A. and Japan. Relatively small variations occur cross-culturally for impression formation from events with transitive behaviors. A fair number of cultural variations have been found in impression formation from copular statements. |
|